References (57)
References
Aoun, J., & Li, Y. A. (1993). On some differences between Chinese and Japanese wh-elements. Linguistic Inquiry, 24, 365–372.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., Christensen, R., Singmann, H., Dai, B., Scheipl, F., Grothendieck, G., & Green, P. (2020). Package ‘lme4’ (Version 1.1–26).Google Scholar
Belikova, A., & White, L. (2009). Evidence for the fundamental difference hypothesis or not? Island constraints revisited. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 31, 199–223. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R. W., Felix, S. W., & Ioup, G. L. (1988). The accessibility of universal grammar in adult language learning. Second Language Research, 4, 1–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cable, S. (2010). The grammar of Q: Q-particles, wh-movement, and pied-piping. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cheung, C.-H. C. (2014). Wh-fronting and the left periphery in Mandarin. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 23, 393–431. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1986). Barriers. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1995). The minimalist program. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2000). Minimalist inquiries: The framework. In R. Martin, D. Michaels, & J. Uriagereka (Eds.), Step by step: Essays on minimalism in honor of Howard Lasnik (pp. 89–155). The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2001). Derivation by phase. In M. Kenstowicz (Ed.), Ken Hale: A life in language (pp. 1–52). The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drummond, A. (2006). Ibex, experiment platform.Google Scholar
Erteschik-Shir, N. (2007). Information structure. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Felser, C. (2001). Wh-expletives and secondary predication: German partial wh-movement reconsidered. Journal of Germanic Linguistics, 13, 5–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goto, N. (2017). Eliminating the strong/weak parameter on T. In M. Y. Erlewine (Ed.), Proceedings of GLOW in Asia XI, (Vol. 2, pp. 57–71. MITWPL.Google Scholar
Hagstrom, P. (1998). Decomposing questions (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). MIT.
Hawkins, R., & Chan, C. Y-H. (1997). The partial availability of Universal Grammar in second language acquisition: The ‘failed functional feature hypothesis’. Second Language Research, 13, 187–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Hattori, H. (2006). Interpretation of English multiple wh-question by Japanese speakers: A missing uninterpretable feature account. Second Language Research, 22, 269–301. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huang, C.-T. J. (1982). Logical relations in Chinese and the theory of grammar (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). MIT.
Johnson, J., & Newport, E. (1991). Critical period effects on universal properties of language: The status of subjacency in the acquisition of a second language. Cognition, 39, 215–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kimura, T. (2022a). Feature selection, feature reassembly, and the role of Universal Grammar: The acquisition of wh-questions by Japanese and Chinese learners of English (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Chuo University.
(2022b). Reassembly of uninterpretable features in L2 acquisition: Evidence from wh-questions. In Y. Gong & F. Kpogo (Eds.), Proceedings of the 46th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 378–389. Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Kuno, S. (1973). The structure of the Japanese language. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kuznetsova, A, Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lardiere, D. (2008). Feature assembly in second language acquisition. In J. Liceras, H. Zobl, & H. Goodluck (Eds.), The role of formal features in second language acquisition (pp. 106–140). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Law, P. (2010). Scrambling of wh-phrases in Japanese. In R. Otoguro, K. Ishikawa, H. Umemoto, K. Yoshimoto, & Y. Harada (Eds.), Proceedings of the 24th Pacific Asia Conference on Language, Information and Computation[URL] (pp. 463–470). Waseda University, Tohoku University.
Liu, Y. (2019). Explicating wh-fronting in Mandarin: A scrambling approach. In A. G. Grognet (Ed.), Proceedings of Linguistics Society of America (Vol. 43, pp. 1–12). LSA. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martohardjono, G. (1993). Wh-movement in the acquisition of a second language: A cross-linguistic study of three languages with and without movement (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Cornell University.
Miyamoto, Y., & Iijima, Y. (2003). On the existence of scrambling in the grammar of Japanese elementary EFL learners. In S. Foster-Cohen & S. P. Doehler (Eds.) Eurosla Yearbook 3, (pp. 7–27). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nishigauchi, T. (1986). Quantification in syntax (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
Reinhart, T. (1981). A second COMP position. In A. Belletti, L. Brandi, & L. Rizzi (Eds.), Theory of markedness in generative grammar (pp. 517–557). Scuola Normale Superiore di Pisa.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (2004). Locality and left periphery. In A. Beletti (Ed.) Structures and beyond: The cartography of syntactic structures (Vol. 3, pp. 223–251). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ross, J. (1967). Constraints on variables in syntax (Unpublished doctoral dissertation) MIT.
Saito, M. (1992). Long distance scrambling in Japanese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 1, 69–118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003). A derivational approach to the interpretation of scrambling chains. Lingua, 113, 481–518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). (A) Case for labeling: Labeling in languages without ϕ-feature agreement. The Linguistic Review, 33, 129–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). Japanese wh-phrases as operators with unspecified quantificational force. Language and Linguistics, 18, 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sauerland, U., & Elbourne, P. (2002). Total reconstruction, PF movement and derivational order. Linguistic Inquiry, 33, 283–319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schachter, J. (1989). Testing a proposed universal. In S. Gass & J. Schachter (Eds.), Linguistic perspectives on second language acquisition (pp. 73–88). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B. D., & Sprouse, R. A. (1996). L2 cognitive states and the Full Transfer/Full Access model. Second Language Research, 12, 40–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sharwood-Smith, M. (1988). On the role of linguistic theory in explanations of second language developmental grammars. In S. Flynn & W. O’Neil (Eds.), Linguistic theory in second language acquisition (pp. 173–198). Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shimoyama, J. (2006). Indeterminate phrase quantification in Japanese. Natural Language Semantics, 14, 139–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Starke, M. (2001). Move dissolves into Merge: A theory of locality (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Geneva.
Szabolcsi, A., & Zwarts, F. (1993). Weak islands and an algebraic semantics of scope taking. Natural Language Semantics, 1, 235–284. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsai, W.-T. D. (1999). On lexical courtesy. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 8, 39–73. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M. (2003). Clitics and determiners in L2 Greek. In J. M. Liceras, H. Zobl, & H. Goodluck (Eds.), Proceedings of the 6th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference (GASLA 2002) (pp. 331–339). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., & Dimitrakopoulou, M. (2007). The Interpretability Hypothesis: Evidence from wh-interrogatives in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 23, 215–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., & Roussou, A. (1991). Parameter-resetting in L2? UCL Working Papers in Linguistics, 3, 149–70.Google Scholar
University of Cambridge Local Examinations Syndicate. (2001). Quick Placement Test. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Uriagereka, J. (1999). Multiple spell-out. In S. Epstein & N. Hornstein (Eds.), Working minimalism (pp. 251–282). The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wakabayashi, S. (1997). The acquisition of functional categories by learners of English (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Cambridge.
(2002). The acquisition of non-null subjects in English: A minimalist account. Second Language Research, 18, 28–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wakabayashi, S., & Okawara, I. (2003). Japanese learners’ errors on long distance wh-questions. In S. Wakabayashi (Ed.), Generative approaches to the acquisition of English by native speakers of Japanese (pp. 215–46). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Watanabe, A. (1992). Subjacency and S-structure movement of wh-in-situ. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 1, 255–292. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L. (1992). Subjacency violations and empty categories in L2 acquisition. In H. Goodluck & M. Rochemont (Eds.), Island constraints (pp. 445–464). Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003). Second language acquisition and Universal Grammar. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wu, J. (1999). Syntax and semantics of quantification in Chinese (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Maryland.
Yuan, B., & Dugarova, E. (2012). Wh-topicalization at the syntax-discourse interface in English speakers’ L2 Chinese grammars. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34, 533–560. DOI logoGoogle Scholar